No. But this is what happens when a flawed scientific experiment is combined with generous helpings of public-relations nonsense.
Richard Chi and Allan Snyder from the University of Sydney have claimed that a form of brain
stimulation known as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can help
people “escape the tricks our minds impose on us”. An extraordinary piece of PR
puffery from by the University of Sydney headlines with: 'Impossible' problem solved after non-invasive brain stimulation.
These are precisely the
kinds of claims that damage public
understanding of science. I'm getting deja vu just writing this.
In their latest
outing, published in the journal Neuroscience Letters, Chi and Snyder asked whether tDCS could help people solve a puzzle
called the 'nine dots problem', which requires people to connect a 3 x 3 grid of
dots using four straight lines. They report that participants who received tDCS of their anterior temporal lobes were better able to solve the
puzzle than those who had ‘sham’ stimulation. They conclude that this effect
was “due to inhibiting [brain] networks associated with top down imposition
of prior knowledge, knowledge that inclines us to join the dots up within the
square.”
Really? I've read their paper several times now, back to front, even sideways a couple of times. And I still can't find any evidence to support this claim. Instead all I found was a long list of flaws. Here are some of the worst offenders.
1. No control task for effects of "prior knowledge"
To conclude that tDCS
disrupted the imposition of “prior knowledge”, they would need to include a
control task that is matched on every possible dimension (task difficulty, attentional
requirements, etc.) but which isn’t prone to the imposition of “prior knowledge”. They didn’t. They also fail to
provide any cogent theoretical framework for what neural mechanisms are
responsible for “imposing prior knowledge”. In the Discussion, the authors seem to acknowledge this lack of experimental control: "While there could well be alternative explanations, we
would like to emphasize that our results that tDCS enabled over 40% of
participants to solve the nine-dot problem stand on their own." I beg to differ.
2. Actually, no suitable control tasks at all
Participants could
have performed better because tDCS increased their alertness or arousal, or
altered their attentional state in any one of many possible ways. But the authors included no control tasks to test whether these processes were affected, either positively or
negatively.
3. Claims based on mysterious pilot data
The authors describe in
handwaving fashion a pilot study that compared different forms of brain
stimulation, and then conclude that this pilot study confirms their previous
findings. But the results of the pilot study are not shown and are not
even statistically analysed. Did the reviewers of this paper even read it?
4. Dubious statistical analysis
Their use of one-tailed significance testing is highly questionable. A
directional prediction alone is not sufficient justification for one-tailed
tests. This is is because one-tailed testing is only legitimate if the opposite result to that expected is either impossible or irrelevant. Neither is the case here, or arguably ever in cognitive neuroscience: had
brain stimulation impaired rather than enhanced performance then the authors
would naturally still have interpreted their data. The authors also appear
confused about the nature of probabilities. For the key analysis, comparing the
number of participants who solved the puzzle between the sham and experimental
groups, they report a p
value of .018, and then conclude bizarrely that the likelihood of observing the
results by chance was less than one in 10,000. Pardon?
5. Bizarre inclusion of additional participants
The actual experiment
was undertaken on 22 participants. Eleven of these participants were in the
control group (they received ‘sham’ tDCS in which the current is switched off
soon after being started), and the other eleven were in the tDCS group. But in
the Results, the authors suddenly add another 11 participants to the sample
without providing any detail of the experimental procedures.
6. I don't even know what to call this problem. Appeal to...prose?
Part of the results
consists of an anecdote of what one participant had to say about his "cognitive style". This section is rather optimistically titled “Case Report”.
I am honestly struggling
to find any reasonable explanation for how this paper could have survived even
a modestly critical peer-review process. I could give this paper to any one of my PhD students and they would find more flaws than I've even listed here. But perhaps I shouldn’t be surprised, as Snyder's group have previously published a similarly uncontrolled study,
combined with a healthy dose of exaggeration and media hype. I wonder if the Editor
of Neuroscience Letters would care to
enlighten us on what went wrong this time.
For my part, I would advise scientists,
journalists and the public to treat this piece of research by Chi and Snyder with
extreme caution.
The press office at
the University of Sydney also has much to answer for. Many of us in the
scientific community are striving to improve the quality of science news by ensuring
that we avoid exaggeration and hyperbole in our dealings with the media. It helps nobody when press offices
produce cynical puff pieces with stupid, sensationalistic headlines.