A lot of enjoyable things in
life are risky and potentially addictive. So how do we control our impulses?
And why do some people find it harder to say ‘no’ than others?
In a recent study we asked whether
a key to self-control could lie in an unexpected place: a corner of our cognitive
system that controls motor actions. We found that when people did a simple task
that required starting and stopping finger movements, they also took less risk when
gambling. This effect lasted at least two hours after being trained in so-called
‘motor inhibition’.
Why should the act of inhibiting
simple movements lead to more cautious gambling behaviour? We don't yet know, but our working
hypothesis is that it boosts or primes an inhibition system in the brain that regulates a range of functions - including complex decision-making. By strengthening
motor inhibition through the mental equivalent of a ‘gym workout’ we may be able to open new
avenues for treating problem gambling and other addictions.
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Source article: Verbruggen,
F., Adams, R., & Chambers, C.D. (2012). Proactive motor control reduces
monetary risk taking in gambling. Psychological
Science, 23, 805-815. [pdf] [press release]
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Imagine
the following scenario. You are driving to meet your financial adviser for a
meeting about your investments. Along the way you encounter a series of obstacles
that cause you to drive with extra caution: roadworks, speed cameras, and intermittent
bursts of rain. When you eventually arrive and sit down with your adviser, she
asks how you would like to spread your reserves between a number of low- and
high-risk options. Choosing isn’t easy – the higher risk investments could pay
for that much-needed vacation in the Maldives, but the market is unpredictable
and you could lose out. You make your choices.
Clearly
this decision is complex and based on many different sources of information.
But ask yourself: would your decision have been the same if the journey to the
meeting had been free of obstacles? Intuitively, you’re probably thinking “Huh?
I would select my investments rationally, why should the drive there make any
difference?” And most people would agree with you – society reinforces the
notion that being able to make decisions rationally and without bias is part of
what ‘makes us human’.
There’s
just one problem with this argument: it doesn’t quite fit the evidence. Previous
research tells us that multitasking
impairs cognition, and we also know that priming
people in various ways can bias social attitudes and financial decisions that we would intuitively
ascribe to our free will. A recent study, for example,
found that priming people with the mere image of a thinking man reduced their religious beliefs. At the same time, taxing
self-control can cause what social psychologists call ego depletion, reducing our ability to
resist temptation.
So,
are you still sure that your cautious driving would have no effect on your
investment decisions?
Spreading caution around
If our
ability to make rational decisions can be influenced by cognitive interference,
then you might assume that such effects should impair decision-making. Some evidence does indeed suggest that
taxing executive control can make it harder for
people to inhibit impulsive choices, although not all studies agree.
But what
if we could specifically tailor a kind of multitasking that would improve your decision-making? In other
words, what if the interference somehow biased you to take less risk, like the example above with cautious driving? To test
this idea, we designed a laboratory task that brings together two different
forms of decision-making: monetary gambling and basic stopping of a motor
response.
Here’s
how it worked. On each trial of the task, people were presented with six
options below a series of yellow bars. Each of these options was a number of
points that could be won, which – depending on the condition – ranged from 2 to
448. Higher amounts were intuitively more attractive but, crucially, also had a
lower chance of winning. And if you did lose the gamble then you forfeited half
the amount wagered. So, for instance, if you picked ‘112’ you had only a 15% chance
of winning the 112 points, but an 85% chance of losing 66 points. Whereas if
you picked ‘2’ you had a 75% chance of winning those 2 points, and only a 25%
change of losing 1 point.
We
didn’t tell people the exact probabilities of winning or losing, but we did
tell them that the chances of winning were lower for higher amounts. We
then calculated a simple betting score by taking the average of the choices across
the options: from 1 to 6, ranked in order of lowest risk to highest risk. This
means that the higher the score, the more willing the participant was to take
risks when gambling. At the end of the experiment participants were paid the
overall amount they won, at a rate of 1000 points to £1.
On
each trial of this task, participants were given a few seconds to reach a decision
before the yellow bars started rising toward a white line. Once the bars
reached the line they then pressed whichever key corresponded to their choice. This
was followed by feedback as to how many points they won or lost on that trial, plus
a readout of their overall points balance.
To
test how stopping of simple responses (i.e. motor
inhibition) interacts with gambling decisions, we introduced an additional
catch. Sometimes the bars would turn black just before reaching the white line.
On these trials, participants were told to stop whatever decision they had planned.
If they stopped successfully then they would win points, but if they failed to
stop they would lose points.
The critical
manipulation in this experiment was the expectation
of stopping. To achieve this we further split the task into blocks of
trials in which participants either expected ‘stop signals’ to occasionally occur
(dual-task blocks, so named because
these blocks included two tasks, gambling and stopping) or in which they were
told in advance that signals would never occur (single-task blocks, so named because these blocks only included the
gambling task).
We
then compared the average betting scores between the blocks, focusing
specifically on the trials without
stop-signals. This allowed us to directly compare the effect on gambling of
either expecting or not expecting to stop a response, while keeping everything
else the same. In other words, the only thing that differed between these two
conditions was the participant’s cognitive expectations.
So
what did we predict would happen? There are two main possibilities. On the one
hand, when people were in dual-task blocks they were now dividing their
attention between two tasks. It is possible that this state of divided
attention and cognitive ‘load’ could interfere with decision-making in the
gambling task, making it harder for people to resist the more tempting,
higher-risk options. We called this hypothesis the interference account.
On
the other hand, we also know that when people expect to stop a response they
become more cautious in their motor control – mainly, they slow down. So could this state of motor
cautiousness transfer or spread to other forms of decision-making? If so, then when
people expect to stop their response in the dual-task blocks, they might
actually become more cautious and so
take less risk than in the single-task blocks. We called this hypothesis the transfer account.
So which
hypothesis won in the contest between interference and transfer? The results
clearly supported the transfer account. When people expected to stop their motor
response, their betting score decreased by 10-15% compared with when they knew
they wouldn’t have to stop. So when people were expecting that they might have
to stop their response, they freely chose
to place less risky bets.
To be
sure that this effect was specific to motor inhibition,
rather than attention or other general effects of cognitive load, we also tested another
group of participants in a ‘double-response’ control condition. Rather than stopping their response on signal
trials, participants in the double-response group made an extra response. The double-response group showed no such reduction
in risky gambling (in fact, it increased slightly), which helps tie the effects
in the stop group to inhibition. And to be sure that these findings weren’t a statistical fluke, we ran the whole experiment
twice in different participants to replicate the main finding.
What
do these results signify? From a theoretical perspective they reveal an overlap
between different forms of inhibition: inhibition of motor responses causally shaped
inhibition of risky gambling decisions. Previous studies have hinted at that
such links might exist but much of this evidence relies on correlation rather
than causation. For instance, people
with a gambling addiction can sometimes show
impairments in motor inhibition but it is unclear whether
these problems are causally related.
Having
uncovered evidence for a causal link we next asked whether training people in
motor inhibition could have a more lasting effect. If so, this would suggest
that the relationship between motor inhibition and risk-taking behaviour might
be developed as a complementary therapy for addiction.
Bootcamp for inhibition?
In
the next series of experiments we asked whether training people to stop
responses could reduce risk-taking later in time. The idea was to train people
for a short period (about 30 minutes) at motor inhibition, followed by monetary
gambling. The gambling task was the same as described above but including the single-task
blocks only, i.e. the bars never turned black and participants never expected
to stop their responses while gambling.
We
began by dividing people into three training groups. The stop group did a standard motor inhibition task, called the
stop-signal task. The double-response
group did a different (non-inhibition) task on the same stimuli. The control group didn’t do any training – they
just skipped straight to the gambling task.
The stop-signal task is a workhorse of
experimental psychology made famous by Gordon Logan, and one of the most straightforward
and elegant tests of cognitive function. In our version of the
task, participants were shown a shape on a computer screen (square or
diamond) and were asked to identify the shape as quickly as possible by
pressing one of two buttons, e.g. left button for a square vs. right button for a diamond.
On a
random third of trials, the shape turned bold after a short delay. These trials
are called ‘signal trials’ and the participant is instructed to try and stop
their response. Successfully stopping your response is easy when the signal
occurs immediately after the shape appears, but it becomes progressively more
difficult as the delay between shape and the stop-signal is increased. This is
because, at longer delays, you will be closer to executing your initial
response by the time the signal occurs, so there is less time to countermand
that response.
Our double-response group did a control task
on the same stimuli: instead of trying to stop their response on signal trials,
they instead executed a second response. So their task had similar attentional
demands as the stop-signal task, but crucially without requiring motor inhibition.
So
what might happen if we give participants the stop-signal task followed by the
gambling task? If the effect of motor inhibition transfers over time to risk-taking
behaviour then we expected training to make people more cautious in their gambling
decisions, producing a similar effect to the first series of experiments. On
the other hand, requiring people to continuously start and stop for 30 minutes might
fatigue their inhibitory
control and lead to increased risk-taking.
Once
again the results were clear: motor inhibition training reduced risky gambling
by 10-15%. Interestingly, we saw the same pattern even when we introduced a
2-hour gap between the end of the stop training and the start of the gambling
task.
A picture takes shape…
To
summarise, we found that when people expected they might have to stop a motor
response in a gambling task, they opted for less risky choices. And when we
trained people to stop motor responses before
doing the same gambling task, they also selected less risky options. This post-training
aftereffect lasted for at least two hours. Overall then, these results indicate
that these very different types of cognitive control are tightly coupled.
Why such
a link, you might ask. One possibility is that motor inhibition and risky decision-making
draw on the same regulatory systems in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), a complex and mysterious
part of the brain that coordinates a range of executive functions.
Of
course, since these experiments are purely psychological, we can’t draw any
conclusions about what might be changing in the DLPFC, but there are several possibilities
to consider in future studies. For instance, recent work has found that more impulsive people tend to have lower levels of an inhibitory
neurotransmitter called GABA in their DLPFC. Could motor cautiousness
and inhibition training be somehow altering the expression of GABA in the DLPFC?
Is motor cautiousness somehow tuning neural networks that regulate our
behaviour, strengthening or biasing a computational ‘muscle’ that is used for
decision-making? Perhaps inhibition training boosts the activity of DLPFC in
regulating more primitive parts of the brain that respond to emotion and reward,
such as the amygdala? Such questions are speculative,
so to learn more we are now combining motor inhibition and gambling with a
range of neuroscience methods, including transcranial
magnetic stimulation (TMS), fMRI, simultaneous TMS-fMRI, and magnetic resonance
spectroscopy.
As
well as helping us understand more about cognitive control, our findings also
have possible implications for treating gambling addiction. Related work by Katrijn Houben and Anita Jansen suggests that motor
inhibition is linked to other compulsive behaviours, such as overeating and
alcohol consumption. So could a regime of motor inhibition training help people
overcome addiction? It seems possible, but we can’t claim from our results that
motor inhibition provides a cure or treatment for any addiction. It is important
to stress that all of the experiments in our study included healthy people only,
and we currently have no data on whether motor inhibition training has any beneficial
effect in a clinical situation. Furthermore, the effects we found are modest,
just a 10-15% reduction in risk-taking. That said, we think the clinical angle
is worth exploring and we may be able to tweak the design to make these effects
larger and more clinically significant.
So
can motor inhibition help us resist temptation? Possibly, yes. The next
challenge is to figure out why and explore the implications – and applications
– in clinical psychology and psychiatry.
____
* All comments and questions are
welcome.
* The press release associated with
this study follows a new format arising from the recent Royal Institution debate
we took part in on science and the media, hosted by Alok Jha and Alice Bell, and
also featuring Ed Yong, Fiona Fox, and Ananyo Bhattacharya.
This is a really impressive series of experiments, with a very compelling result and clear implications - well done! And a great lay write-up, I hardly need to go and read the original :-)
ReplyDeleteLooking forward to hearing how the next series of experiments progress, care of the ESRC. Really well deserved funding.